Education, Science, Technology, Innovation and Life
Open Access
Sign In

Power and Automatic Cognitive Processing

Download as PDF

DOI: 10.23977/appep.2024.050116 | Downloads: 7 | Views: 157

Author(s)

Mufan Zheng 1

Affiliation(s)

1 Department of Psychology, Wuhan University, Wuhan, China

Corresponding Author

Mufan Zheng

ABSTRACT

This article discussed how power affects individuals’ cognitive processing style. Power increases the tendency to rely on automatic cognitive processing during impression formation. Compared to low power individuals or groups, those with high power tend to pay less attention to subordinates, to use stereotyping and prejudices, and to make less accurate judgments. Control motivation and approach/inhibition tendency provide theoretical explanation for the effect. There still lacks further evidences to show that power affects cognitive processing in more broad scope besides impression formation. Goal-oriented theory and abstractive thinking may provide a new perspective to understand the link of power and cognitive processing style.

KEYWORDS

Power, cognitive processing style, automatic cognition

CITE THIS PAPER

Mufan Zheng, Power and Automatic Cognitive Processing. Applied & Educational Psychology (2024) Vol. 5: 116-120. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.23977/appep.2024.050116.

REFERENCES

[1] Goodwin S A, Gubin A, Fiske S T, et al. Power can bias impression processes: stereotyping subordinates by default and by design[J]. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 2000, 3(3): 227-256.
[2] Fiske S T. Controlling other people. The impact of power on stereotyping [J]. American Psychologist, 1993, 48(6): 621-628.
[3] Keltner D, Gruenfeld D H, Anderson C. Power, approach, and inhibition[J]. Psychological Review, 2003, 110(2): 265-284.
[4] Guinote A, Willis G B, Martellotta C. Social power increases implicit prejudice[J]. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2010, 46(2): 299-307.
[5] See K E, Morrison E W, Rothman N B, et al. The detrimental effects of power on confidence, advice taking, and accuracy [J]. Organizational Behavior & Human Decision Processes, 2011, 116(2): 272-285.
[6] Wegner D M, Bargh J A. Control and automaticity in social life[C]. In: Gilbert, D., Fiske, S. T., Lindzey, G., Ed., Handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 2). New York: McGraw-Hill, 1998: 446-496.
[7] Newman J P (1997). Conceptual models of the nervous system: Implications for antisocial behavior[C]. In: Stoff, D. M., Breiling, J., Maser, J. D, Ed., Handbook of antisocial behavior (pp 324–335). New York: Wiley, 1997: 324-335
[8] Higgins E T. (1999). Promotion and prevention as motivational duality: Implications for evaluative processes[C]. In: Chaiken, S., Trope, Y., Ed., Dual-process theories in social psychology. New York: Guilford Press, 1999: 503-525.
[9] Lerner J S, Keltner D. Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion specific influences on judgment and choice[J]. Cognition and Emotion, 2000, 14(4): 473–493.
[10] Weick M, Guinote A. When subjective experiences matter: power increases reliance on the ease of retrieval[J]. Journal of personality and social psychology, 2008, 94(6): 956.
[11] Richeson J A, Ambady N. Effects of situational power on automatic racial prejudice[J]. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2003, 39(2): 177-183.
[12] Keltner D, Robinson R J. Defending the status quo: power and bias in social conflict[J]. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 1997, 23(10): 1066-1077.
[13] Kipnis, D. Does power corrupt? [J]. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 1972, 24(24): 33-41.
[14] Klocke, U. "I am the best": effects of influence tactics and power bases on powerholders' self and target-evaluations [J]. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 2009, 12: 619-637.
[15] Guinote A, Brown M, Fiske S T. Minority status decreases sense of control and increases interpretive processing [J].Social Cognition, 2006, 24(2): 169-186.
[16] Golby A J, Gabrieli J D, Chiao J Y, Eberhardt J L. Differential responses in the fusiform region to same-race and other-race faces[J]. Nature Neuroscience, 2001, 4(8): 845-850.
[17] Park B, Ryan C S, Judd C M. Role of meaningful subgroups in explaining differences in perceived variability for in-groups and out-groups[J]. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 1992, 63(4): 553-567.
[18] Overbeck J R, Park B. When power does not corrupt: superior individuation processes among powerful perceivers [J].Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 2001, 81(4): 549-565.
[19] Smith P K, Trope Y. You focus on the forest when you're in charge of the trees: power priming and abstract information processing [J]. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 2006, 90(4): 578-596. 
[20] Guinote A, Vescio T K. The situated focus theory of power[C]. In: Guinote, A., Vescio, T. K., Ed., The social psychology of power. New York: Guilford Press, 2010:141-173.

All published work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

Copyright © 2016 - 2031 Clausius Scientific Press Inc. All Rights Reserved.